首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   238篇
  免费   24篇
  国内免费   31篇
航空   95篇
航天技术   33篇
综合类   26篇
航天   139篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   3篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   12篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   29篇
  2013年   55篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   9篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   10篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   17篇
  1999年   18篇
  1997年   1篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1992年   2篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   2篇
排序方式: 共有293条查询结果,搜索用时 671 毫秒
231.
提出一种基于对称均匀线阵的波达方向(DOA)估计新方法.该算法在未知噪声协方差矩阵为复对称Toeplitz结构的情况下,利用空间差分方法和相干信源信息Toeplitz矩阵重构相结合,来处理同时存在相干、非相干信源的情况.该算法在提高阵列的信源过载能力的同时,可明显减小运算量.计算机仿真结果证明了算法的有效性和正确性.  相似文献   
232.
An ionospheric spatial gradient represents the ionosphere delay difference between different locations, and its variation over a specific area is important for implementing differential GNSS systems. An estimation method for the ionospheric spatial gradient over a small regional area is proposed. A plate map model is implemented for the direct estimation of the gradients. Nine years of GPS data were processed to figure out the annual variation of the mean gradient at the mid-geomagnetic latitude of 30° N. Gradients along the north–south direction have a mean of 0.65 mm/km and follow solar-cycle variations.  相似文献   
233.
This study employed an information accumulation model of choice reaction times to investigate alignment effects in mental representations of maps. University students studied a map from a single orientation (with north at the top). In a subsequent two-choice reaction time task, the students’ spatial knowledge of the map was assessed employing spatial left/right judgments, which were made from imagined perspectives that were either north-aligned or south-aligned. Data showed a standard alignment effect, favoring north- over south-aligned trials. To examine the locus of this effect, data were fit using the Linear Ballistic Accumulator (LBA) model of speeded decisions (Brown & Heathcote, 2008). Of interest were three model parameters: drift rate, the speed at which evidence accumulates toward a response; response threshold, the amount of evidence demanded from the decision maker before selecting a response; and non-decision time, the time consumed by pre- and postdecisional processes. The best-fitting model suggested that non-decision time accounted for the alignment effect. The difference in non-decision time between north and south-aligned judgments suggests a mental alignment stage on south-aligned trials, accounting for the longer reaction times for judgements misaligned with the presented north orientation of the map.  相似文献   
234.
Research on location memory suggests that integration of separate sources of information does not occur when recalling the position of a common target object. In a relatively simple task, previous research shows no observable benefit from holding two spatial memories compared to one. It has been suggested that exclusively utilizing only one of two memories may account for this finding. The current research tests the robustness of this idea as well as an alternative in the form of an averaging approach to combining spatial information. The results suggest that exclusivity may not be the best account for performance of multiple object-location memories. Rather, memories may well combine in a manner similar to averaging, where information is available for each memory but combined in a nonbeneficial way.  相似文献   
235.
People often communicate with reference to informally agreed places, such as “the city centre”. However, views of the spatial extent of such areas may vary, resulting in imprecise regions. We compare perceptions of Sheffield’s City Centre from a street survey to extents derived from various web-based sources. Such automated approaches have advantages of speed, cost and repeatability. We show that footprints from web sources are often in concordance with models derived from more labour-intensive methods. Notable exceptions however were found with sources advertising or selling residential property. Agreement between sources was measured by aggregating them to identify locations of consensus.  相似文献   
236.
Movement experts tend to outperform non-experts on some tasks of spatial ability, suggesting that movement experts possess enhanced spatial-cognitive abilities, which may be developed over years of practice. In the current study, movement experts (dancers and athletes) and non-experts completed one verbal working memory task and two spatial working memory tasks—a traditional Corsi block-tapping task and a new full-bodied version of the Corsi task, nicknamed the “Twister Task.” Movement experts outperformed non-experts on both the Corsi and Twister tasks but not on the verbal task, suggesting that movement experience may relate to spatial working memory specifically. Additionally, the Twister task significantly correlated with the traditional Corsi task, providing validation for a new measure of spatial working memory.  相似文献   
237.
传统的空间谱估计测向模型中没有考虑来波极化这一维度的信息,将其用于极化敏感阵列进行测向时会导致测向的灵敏度和精度下降。为此,对传统的空间谱估计测向模型进行了修正,加入对来波极化信息的考虑,提出一种极化敏感阵列的空间谱估计测向技术,并对该技术的可行性进行了研究。给出极化敏感阵列的空间谱估计测向模型,在此基础上,采用M USIC算法进行测向,并对影响测向精度的因素进行仿真。仿真结果表明,提出的极化敏感阵列空间谱估计测向技术可以实现极化敏感阵列的测向,所得结果对极化敏感阵列的测向问题具有重要意义。  相似文献   
238.
提出了相干信源DOA估计的非对称加权空间平滑差分(UWSSD)算法。在空间平滑差分运算的基础上,对各子阵协方差矩阵采用非对称权值加权处理,破坏差分矩阵的负反对称特性,从而使信源差分协方差矩阵恢复为满秩。差分矩阵中不含相关噪声及非相关源信息,可利用它分辨相关及相干信源,而利用传统子空间算法分辨非相干信源,从而重复利用阵列接收数据,可分辨更多信源。通过分析差分矩阵的特点,提出了一种无需矩阵特征分解的快速算法。仿真结果表明,UWSSD算法具有较强的噪声抑制能力及信源过载能力。  相似文献   
239.
This study presents two experiments that examine howindividuals learn relative directions betweenlandmarks in a desktop virtual environment. Subjectswere presented snapshot images of different virtualenvironments containing distinguishing landmarks anda road network. Following the presentation of eachvirtual environment, subjects were given a relativedirection test. The relative direction test involvedindicating the direction of hidden landmarks fromdifferent vantage points in the environment. Half ofthese vantage points were presented during thelearning phase, while the other half were novel.Results showed that subjects learned relativedirections between landmarks equally well when sceneswere presented in either a sequential or random order.Furthermore, viewing a configuration of landmarks ina desktop virtual environment from multipleperspectives produced a viewpoint dependentrepresentation in memory. Subjects had significantlygreater response times for new viewing perspectives,as compared to previously viewed scenes. Thisviewpoint dependent representation of the environmentpersisted despite learning under conditions ofspatio-temporal discontinuity and changes to anenvironmental frame of reference.  相似文献   
240.
Abstract

Spatial proximity terms, such as near and far, communicate information regarding the distance in which a “located” object can be found with respect to a “reference” object. The present paper investigates whether people take into account the location of an object extraneous to the located object and reference object pair, when setting the scale for proximity language judgements. Across three experiments participants rated the appropriateness of near and far to describe spatial scenes that included a third (distractor) object positioned the same distance as the located object from the reference object, but at varying distances from the located object. The results show that the presence of other spatial relations affects scale setting, resulting in differences in appropriateness ratings for those spatial terms.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号